71 ILLNESS AND INTERACTION: A CASE OF PARANOIA Atwood D. Gaines Department of Anthropology San Francisco State University San Francisco, California Social interaction is a reflexive, interpretive, and adjustive pro- cess (Mead 1964; Schultz 1967). Social interactionists view the matrix of social life as grounded in everyday social interactional processes, in the meanings brought to, exchanged and derived from social inter- course (Blumer 1969). Interaction, whether among the unacquainted or the acquainted, is viewed as patterned by interactionists. The patterning of interpersonal behavior is seen as deriving from a learned and shared set of implicit assumptions which form the basis for a grammar of social behavior. As a grammar of a language constitutes a set of rules governing its use, so each culture has assumptions about self, humankind, and society from which social interactional rules are generated. The assumptions of the social games (Bailey 1971) must be learned and employed if one is to participate in social life as a community member in good standing. Studies of social interaction have traditionally been concerned with the analysis of regularities in social interaction as they occur in various societies (Goffman 1959; Bailey 1971). Some studies have con- cerned violations of such interactional norms. Some of Garfinkel's studies (1967) exemplify this approach. Further, Garfinkel (1967) suggests that the assumed interactions of everyday life are actually creative accomplishments, since the success of these interactions is actually problematic, requiring artful negotiation in the accomplishment of seemingly mundane engagements. The subject of this paper is the social interaction of a person ex- periencing an acute paranoid episode. In interactional encounters, the subject continually violated an overarching definition of the situation, that of negotiation, held by other actors. That is, the subject had de- fined all face engagements as opportunities for the discovery of the nature of, and the characters involved in, a plot against him. Thus, while normal interaction is a reflexive, interpretive and adjustive process, our subject failed on all counts. He could not empathize with other's position in interaction, nor could he interpret in an acceptable manner the symbolic meanings presented in interactions. As well, Donald (the subject of this paper) did not adjust himself to the presentation of gestures of others. Donald approached each interaction with a pre- defined stance, took no notice of the managed stances of others, nor of their situation when he accused them of misconduct. He failed to adjust himself to new situations and interactants for, in his eyes, the situa- tions and the actors were all the same. 72 Thus, he violated an overarching assumption of interactants, that each encounter and its meanings, and those which derive from such en- gagements, are all negotiable features of the social process. Donald failed to allow others to negotiate with him the definition of situa- tions or the nature of the social selves they presented. This process of negotiation,and the assumed negotiable character of specific situa- tions allows for the artful accomplishments which are interactional en- counters. I first take up the problem of entree and how I gained rapport with the subject of this paper, Then I consider some of the problems involved in the study. This is followed by a general delineation of the char- acteristics of persecutory paranoia relevant to the study of social in- teraction. The remainder of the paper considers the social interaction of the subject in private and public places and in unfocused and focused interactional encounters from a dramaturgical perspective. ENTREE The subject, referred to as Donald, is a former teacher in his early fifties. Donald believed the author to be endangered, as he was, by a persecuting community of malevolent plotters. This group of persons is called a pseudocommunity by clinicians and "is a reconstruction of reality. It organizes the observed and the inferred behavior of real and imagined persons into a conspiracy, with the patient as its focus" (Cameron 1963:486). The author could be trusted more because he too was an object of the plotters than because of our very long friendship. It is probably true, however, that this friendship gave rise to Donald's belief that both of us were endangered. I was Donald's teammate, and as a team member, I was allowed to observe the back as well as the front region behavior of Donald. That is, I was privy to the construction of new assumptions in private spheres which Donald used in his public behavior. PROBLEMS OF THE STUDY As Donald's trusted team member, I was expected to perform in con- cert with him. But because I was aware that my team member's perceptions of reality were problematic, I often had to "communicate out of character." In other words, I was forced to assist the other teams, tacitly and tact- fully to allow them to maintain the impression or impressions they were attempting to foster in a given social situation (Goffman 1959:167). I often had to counter my teammate's impressions by entering into collu- sion with the other team or audience in order that they might save their own show. Such behavior mitigated against the aggressive reactions of others and allowed them to be less defensive. This was important be- cause defensive maneuvers were quickly perceived by Donald and interpreted as a defense against the penetration of a veil of silence enclosing the pseudocommunity's team secrets. It is also important to note that de- fenses often take the form of aggressive behavior, precisely the kind of 73 behavior which Donald was anticipating. Such occasions became focal points for the expression of Donald's own aggression which he brought to each interactional encounter. In Coffman's terms, I was sometimes an informer. "The informer is someone who pretends to the performers to be a member of their team,, is allowed to come backstage and to acquire destructive information, and then openly or secretly sells out the show to the audience" (Goffman 1959:145). The sellouts were not, of course, attempts to harm Donald, but rather they were attempts to minimize interactional conflict and dissonance. "Whenever an audience exercises tact, the possibility will arise that the performer will learn that they are being tactfully protected" Goffman 1959:233). When this occurred, Donald immediately concluded that the audience members were trying to "fool" him, i.e., act as though they did not know of, or were not part of, the plot. Further problems also developed due to my own failings. Emotional control would sometimes prevail over cognitive control. Because of the emotional tone of Donald's interaction, I had to maintain, or at least try to maintain, an imperturbable face with which to counter Donald's emotional state. Cameron provides a rationale for use of a presentation- al face. An ability to tolerate suspense and shift perspectives is the product of mature ego-superego development. In an emotional crisis, this ability may be lost even by maturely organized adults. When such a loss occurs, a person's only salvation lies in his being still able to share his fears and suspicions with somebody who is less emotionally involved and can be trusted. The shocked or frightened adult can then treat a trusted confidant as his temporary substitute ego-superego, making use of his confidant's reality-testing, social skills and detached perspectives. Through some such maneuver, he experiences the enormous comfort of sharing his anxiety with someone who is concerned but does not get upset. He gains the advantage of seeing things from cooler, more objective points of view [Cameron 1963:477]. My efforts sometimes failed, but apparently I was generally able to main- tain the proper face (Goffman 1967:5), since I was never rejected or classed with the conspirators. There is a very fine line between detachment and understanding of the paranoiac's conceptual systematization of the world. I could not behave in a manner which reinforced Donald's belief system, nor could I express my disbelief in a threatening or aggressive way. In the first instance, a shared perception of events, actions and behavior serves to 74 reinforce the paranoid's belief system, one of which is already very resilient, and resistant to change. To help the paranoid, it is neces- sary to penetrate the projective system. In the second instance, there is a danger in the fact that either hostility or aggression are perceived by the paranoid as definitive behavioral characteristics of members of the pseudocommunity. Attacking Donald's beliefs, then, would mean to Donald that I was a member of that group. I would then have been unable to help Donald in my capacity as a trusted co-victim. Donald, if he had been left alone to cope with reality, would have continued to build, expand and reinforce his delusional paranoid system. Continual develop- ment of the delusional system would occur because Donald would not re- ceive the perhaps dubious benefit of a differing perceptual viewpoint from a substitute ego-superego, as Cameron's quote explained. This balancing act was made much more difficult by the fact that psychotics like Donald are (sometimes) extremely irritating and frus- trating to deal with. One expects repeated "corrections" of the psychotic's interpretations of events to bring about an instant realization of the falsity of his beliefs and conclusions. Incongruities in conflict with Donald's delusional system were continually glossed over. Everything said had to be phrased in a suggestive rather than a contradictory manner for it was very important to maintain the impression that Donald's percep- tions were possible and not simply ridiculous. I had to understand and see the plausibility of an interpretation without agreeing that it was correct. In dealing with a paranoid, one must fight, in a real sense, to maintain one's own mental equilibrium. This struggle can be easily understood when one realizes that the paranoid's system is not hallucina- tory or illusory, but rather it is a delusional construction of social reality. The things a paranoid sees are always very possibly true: there is always something there, some grain of truth in the paranoid's interpretation. The difference in belief lies only in interpretation, and everyone is aware of their own periodic misinterpretation in their day-to-day interactions. Occasionally, then, I found myself saying, "Maybe Donald was right about that." In fact, the plausibility of paranoid thinking makes it easyto be taken in if one is unsuspecting. Many persecutory paranoids succeed in convincing acquaintances of their stories of persecution (Cameron 1963:484). It is only when one observes the individual social phenomenon upon which a paranoid builds his belief system that one can recognize the problematic nature of his interpretive system. Additional problems in this study were the constraints placed on my own natural behavior. For example, when alone with Donald, it was neces- sary that I exercise caution in what I did and said. Much non-goal behavior had to be eliminated. This elimination was to insure that my behavior was not such that Donald would interpret it as meaning anything other than that which I had consciously intended. The nature of my im- pression management derived from the problems associated with rigidity of interpretation. Conclusions he drew always became facts, not possi- bilities, not probabilities, but incontrovertible facts. 75 A cycle of suspicion,to conclusion,to fact which then became an operational basis for behavior is exemplified by a conclusion Donald came to about his doctor. The situation arose after I had spent some time with him and could suggest he seek help. Before Donald would consent to a visit to a psychiatrist, he insisted that he see his own doctor whom he trusted. After we had seen the physician, I happened to remark "I didn't know he (the doctor) had become completely grey." Donald said that when he first began to see this doctor, "All of his hair was black. This was many years ago, of course." Donald then lapsed into a period of silence, after which Donald said, "I see something here" (a phrase frequently used by Donald which indicated a new insight). "That wasn't my doctor. They put someone up to that"' Donald would not see the doctor again because he had established it as a "fact" that his doctor was being impersonated by one of the conspirators. Another problem in the study came from the ironic fact that very soon Donald's beliefs, like those of other paranoids, came to be true. Paranoid behavior can be bizarre to a degree which results in the scru- tiny of the paranoid by others. Thus people do in fact begin to talk about them because of their suspicious behavior. This required tact to explain to Donald why he occupied the attention of others without implying that he was at all peculiar in behavior or appearance. In making such explanations, I always ran the risk of confirming Donald's suspicions about others and giving rise to the idea that I, too, was a member of this dangerous, plotting community. When such events occurred, I came to doubt my own sense of reality, i.e., sanity, and conversely the "insanity" of Donald. After all, people really came to be in collusion (in an interactional sense) against him. Could I then say he was delusional? In Laing and Esterson (1964) we find that persons who are labeled schizophrenic and who are institutionalized are actually more in touch with the objective reality of their family interactions than were their other "sane" family relations. As in Laing's case history of Maya Abbott (Laing and Esterson 1964:31-50), Donald came to doubt his own sense of mistrust. In Donald's case this mistrust was a sign of recovery; in Maya's case this mistrust was the main precipitating factor of her paranoid schizophrenic condition. (I should note here that Donald showed no schizophrenic symptoms.) The problems in the study, then, had to do with the difficulty of continually and politely telling Donald that his perceptions of reality were problematic while remaining in sympathy with him and his plight. We will now turn to the pertinent general characteristics of para- noids, keeping in mind the striking homology found between textbook paranoids and some of those individuals so labeled. THE PERSECUTORY PARANOID PSYCHOTIC "Paranoid reactions are attempts to escape from tension and anxiety through processes of denial and projection, which result in more or less 76 systematized delusions" (Cameron 1963:473). "The types of paranoia are described according to their content as: megalomaniat persecutory, erotomania, mania of jealousy, etc." (Freud 1920:366). One should not, however, be misled by such categories. Megalomania, as is true with other forms of the condition, is one of many components. It is not an isolate and may be present with a paranoid state labeled as persecutory (Freud 1920:367). For example, often paranoids come to a very logical conclusion (as did Donald) which relates their persecution by numerous and diverse people to their sense of self-worth. That is, if the individual is the object of so much interest and concern, he must be of great importance. The specific label, then, is generally derived from a dominant theme in the individual's condition which is the princi- pal but not isolated theme of the clinical picture. Other aspects of the condition are pertinent here. Paranoid condi- tions, by their evident utilization of the defense mechanism of denial and projection (Freud 1966), are attempts to avoid the destructive effects of previously unconscious impulses and fantasies which intruded on preconscious and conscious psychodynamic organizations. In positing a developmental chronology of defense mechanisms, Anna Freud notes that the "expulsion of ideas or affects from the ego and their relegation to the outside world would be a relief to the ego only when it had learned to distinguish itself from that world" (Freud, Anna 1966:51). The denial of hostility in the self, and its projection into the out- side world forming the delusional pseudocommunity are actually attempts at anxiety reduction and self cure. By placing evil and hostility outside himself, the paranoid is better able to cope with them, for the self is redefined as purely good and a victim of the hostility of others who are seen as purely evil. Because the individual believes himself to be the object of a plot of mammoth proportions, he is often given to solitary ruminations. In these instances, the paranoid searches for, and always finds, confirmations of his beliefs in the actions and words, or lack thereof, of others in past social encounters. These perceived confirmations of the malevolent intentions of others are both real and imagined. But this distinction is not made by the para- noid* Nor is the distinction made between conscious and unconscious attitudes and/or action of others. "It is a striking and generally recognized feature in the behavior of paranoiacs that they attach the greatest significance to trivial details in the behavior of others. Details which are usually overlooked by others they interpret and utilize as the basis of far-reaching con- clusions" (Freud 1938:162),o "The category of the accidental, requiring no motivation, which the normal person lots pass as a part of his own psychic functions and faculty actions, is thus rejected by the paranoiac in his application to the psychic manifestations of others. All that he observes in others is full of meaning; all is explainable" (Freud 193B: 163). 77 The paranoid is hypersensitive to the unconscious traces of hostility, contempt, dislike, indifference or fleeting moods in others. He then magnifies their existence and assumes "that what seems clear to (him) must be equally clear to others. . . . This creates an atmosphere of misunderstanding, questioning, distrust, uneasiness and resentment. Such an atmosphere usually follows the actively paranoid person wherever he goes" (Cameron 1963:478). The meaning of the attribution of consciousness to the behavior of other people plays a large part in the disruption of interactions between the persecutory paranoid and others. Normally such interaction is "a presentation of gestures and a response to the meaning of those gestures" (Mead in Blumer 1969:9). Mead uses gesture in this sense to signify both the expressive and linguistic aspects of interaction. "To make inferences on the basis of incomplete evidence, and even leap to conclusions which may turn out to have been unwarranted, are not necessarily abnormal procedures. It is only when the inferences and the conclusions become fixed, adamant, inflexible, and untouched by contrary evidence that we begin to speak of the delusional thinking" (Cameron 1963:471) characteristic of the paranoid psychotic. The paranoid psychotic sees neither hallucinations nor illusions, but rather has delusionso There is a great difference. The delusions are firmly grounded in social fact; there is something there, but the paranoids' perception of such evidence, however slight, transforms it into further proof of a conspiracy. "In a certain sense, the paranoiac behavior is justified; he perceives something that escapes the normal person; he sees clearer than one of normal intellectual capacity, but his knowledge becomes worthless when he imputes to the others the state of affairs he thus recognizes . . he projects into the mental life of others what exists in his own un- conscious activity" (Freud 1938:163). The persecutory paranoid derives meanings not only as objectively observable products of social interaction, but also from his own sub- jective systems (psychological) of delusion, denial and projection. The paranoid's interpretation is thus highly idiosyncratic. As such, the system of interpretation is not shared by others. The basic interaction- al expectations are breached because its fundamental assumptions are un- shared. We mentioned earlier the hypersensitive character of the paranoid. Coupled with this is an insensitivity of the paranoid to the meaning of his own acts, words and gestures. The consequences are profound. The individual's insensitivity stimulates avoidance by both friends and strangers. Where avoidance is not possible interactants soon develop a dislike for an individual who imputes malevolent motives to them and who clearly feels enmity toward them for no apparent reason. The avoidance and dislike which the paranoid soon begins to experience seem to him to be objective evidence that his construction of reality was and is quite justified. 70 The need for justification of the paranoid's reconstruction of reality is great. The paranoid creates a delusional set of assumptions and characterizations of the motives of others in order that he may express the aggression and hostility he feels toward himself. The per- ceived evil and destructive characteristics of others serve as justifi- cation for the expression of destructive impulses toward them; destruc- tive impulses which, if not directed at others, would be directed at the self. As there is an element of the self-fulfilling prophecy in the para- noid condition, it seems to me that this is actually an unconscious design in the illness. That is, given that the paranoid reaction is an attempt to save the self from the self, it may be the case that there remains a residue of self-destructiveness which seeks gratification throuqh or by means of the delusional system. This notion makes sense given that a paranoid does, in fact, create the very world he fears. One of the methods by which this hostile world is created is the paranoid's compulsive search for clues left intentionally or uninten- tionally by the plotters. The trivial unrelated things noted in daily interaction are connected by the paranoid into an ever-tightening web of intrigue. The lack of basic trust is fundamental here: the paranoid is never secure in interactions with others. Insecurity is manifested by suspicious looks, stares and verbal expressions of mistrust which are all too clearly recognized as such by others. The behavior of others, in turn, is seen by the paranoid as threatening and unpredictable. As a result, the paranoid is always ready to attac , for he feels unjusti- fiab'ly threatened and attacked. "Each individual aligns his action to the action of others by ascer- taining what they are doing or what they intend to do, i.e., getting the meaning of their acts" (Blumer 1969:82). As one can see, the persecu- tory paranoid does not "align his action to the action of others" in the same way others do. His alignment is made only before, but not at the inception of, or during the course of, an encounter. Further, this process of getting the meaning of other's acts is effected by understanding the position of the other individual so one can see oneself as others do and then structuring behavior to fit that view (Mead 196.4). But "above all, he (the paranoid) lacks skill in im- agining himself as being in another person's situation, in taking the other person's role and seeing things from the other person's perspec- tives" (Cameron 1963:500). Unable to see himself as others see him, the paranoid proceeds on a single course in social interaction. This course consists of watchfulness, probing questions and, usually, verbal attack. Social alienation cannot help but result. The paranoid sees such interactional alienation as resulting not from his own actions, but rather from the conscious hostile efforts of others. The paranoid is unable to see himself because all his attention is focused elsewhere as "when one is running away from someone who is chasing him, he is entirely occupied in this action, and his experience may be swallowed up in the objects about him, so that he has, at the time being, no consciousness of self at all" (Mead 1964:207). From Schutz (1967) we learn that normally one's presence "here", in a given sociophychological space, by definition makes a "there" for others. The two perspectives are mutually recognized. There is then a reciprocity of perspectives, an interchangeability of the "here" and the "there" of different egos. This reciprocity is the necessary pre- condition of a shared reality. But in the case of Donald, exchange breaks down. It is obvious that neither the preconditions for the shared reality, nor the shared reality itself are possible for him or others like him. His a priori definition of world and his place in it precludes his understanding of the perspective of others, and, hence their understanding of his. In the foregoing I have delineated certain characteristics of the persecutory paranoid: a faulty perception and interpretation of lin- guistic and verbal meanings, hypersensitivity to the actions of others and insensitivity to his own actions, the use of tentative conclusions as facts, watchfulness, lack of basic trust, lack of reciprocity of perspectives and an unshared social reality. All of these characteris- tics are crucial in affecting the paranoid's interaction. Other impor- tant aspects of the condition, such as the role of sexuality, and the lack of self-esteem, have not been addressed here, since they are not our primary concern. I will now turn to Donald's interaction. The reader should keep in mind the features of the condition from which the individual suffers, especially those enumerated above. To Donald, his social world is full of hostility and aggression which are focused on him. We should remain aware that to Donald the delusions of the hostility, aggression and malevolent intentions of others are not delusions: to him they are social reality. INTERACTION Back Region and Back Reqion Behavior The back region is defined by Goffman (1959:134) as that region "where action occurs that is related to the performance but is inconsis- tent with the appearance fostered by the performance." The back region is private and concealed from front regions where public social inter- actional encounters occur. The back region behavior of Donald is characterized by extreme fear and anxiety. Much aggressive hostility is shown, but only in selected areas of the backstage area. Backstage, i.e., at home, Donald was at the mercy of the pseudocommunity. By being in one place, Donald felt the pseudocommunity could get him. He believed that the house was bugged and the phone tapped. As a consequence, when on the phone or in certain parts of the house (where the bugs were), Donald would talk only r7 n1 of normal things in an effort to manage the impression (cf. Berreman 1962 on impression management) that he was unaware of what they were up to. Donald then neither said nor did anything which would clearly ex- hibit a discrepancy between the fostered impression of not knowing he was being monitored and the reality of his knowing he was. While at home, he would profane the plotters only when he thought he was not being monitored. The statement Donald made, "If they know that I know they are listen- ing, they'll get me. But if they think I don't know, I'll be able to get them'" exemplifies Goffman's concept of information control (1959: 141). Donald was trying to control all three types of destructive in- formation delineated by Goffman (1959:141-145): "dark secrets" which they could use to slander or blackmail him; "strategic secrets" which would allow the plotters to adjust to the next interactional situation; and "inside secrets", those things Donald did not want known by anyone. ("I don't want anyone to know anything about what I know or what I do.") Because Donald felt that others were listening in on his backstage life, he had to act at being normal. Thus, Donald came to perform for himself. As Goffman writes, "A performer may be taken in by his own act, convinced at the moment that the impression of reality which he fosters is the one and only reality. In such cases a performer comes to be his own audience; he comes to be a performer and observer of the same show" (Goffman 1959:80). Ironically, because Donald performed for himself, he came to interact with himself in a way which reinforced his own beliefs of persecution. Also observed in the back region behavior of Donald was ritual profanation, the profanation of the audiences Donald might encounter in the front regions on the "outside." Donald made critical and insulting remarks about everyone he knew and many people he did not know, and imputed the most malicious motives to their actions. These ritual pro- fanations served to dehumanize them, a process which further justified Donald's distrust of and contempt for everyone. Donald's image of others was illustrated by another ritual act: he carried his wallet in his hand at all times while in his home. Stating the reason for this behavior he said if he were not to carry his wallet, "They would steal something out of it or they might even steal the whole thing"' I asked him how he thought someone could enter the house to remove something from the wallet while both he and I were present. Don- ald said, "Well, they have been doing it. Besides, you don't know how devious they are, they can fool you anytime." Another aspect of Donald's back region behavior was his interaction with what Goffman calls service specialists who are a group if "indivi- duals who specialize in the construction, repair, and maintenance of the show their clients maintain before other people" (Goffman 1959:153). A repairman came to Donald's house to repair a gas heater. Donald hovered over him, watching his every move. The repairman said that 31 if he could not fix the heater, it would have to be replaced because it had a leak. Donald interpreted this to mean that the repairman was warning him that the heater, with its escaping gas, was only one of the many methods that they could use to get him. The heater was repaired quickly and the repairman departed. But Donald checked it to make sure that no destructive device had been left behind by the repairman in or around the heater. Donald, however, was still worried about the visit of the service person. He reasoned that even if it were possible that the repairman was not in the plot, he would probably divulge to the plotters informa- tion gleaned during his visit to the house. The plotters could use this new information against him at some future time. Donald's fear illustrates a point made by Goffman. "It would seem that service specialists can hardly attend to the needs of an individual performer without acquiring as much or more destructive information about some aspects of the individual's performance as the individual himself possesses" (Goffman 1951:153). Front Re2ion and Front Region Behavior I will now consider Donald's behavioral transitions from back to front regions. "One of the most interesting times to observe impression management is the moment when a performer leaves the back region and enters the place where the audience is to be found, or when he returns therefrom, for at these moments one can detect a wonderful putting on and taking off of character" (Goffman 1959:121). The back region performance of Donald, which fostered the appearance of unawareness and sought to conceal his anxiety, was carried over into the front region with important additions, watchfulness and readiness to attack. The front region is defined as the location "where a parti- cular performance is or may be in progress" (Goffman 1959:107). We will digress here to discuss some characteristics of Donald in particular and of paranoids in general to clarify some of Donald's prob- lems in interaction. These difficulties in interaction will be dis- cussed shortly. The paranoid comes to interaction with preconceived notions and definitions, i.e., his own reality which is not shared by others. In- teraction, then, involves more than communication about a given face engagement. It is an opportunity to confront a suspected plotter. Donald, prior to an engagement, had defined the kinds of information which he could get during interaction. He proceeded with a single intent, that of gleaning confirmation of his preconceived notions of persecution. For Donald, any given interactional situation was not defined in terms of its actual unique setting(s), participants, and communicated information (both expressive and linguistic), but rather in terms of a nonadjusting projected system of persecutory beliefs. To protect himself, and to catch the plotters, Donald tried to manage an impression of being unaware. This act of impression management 82 was, however, extremely poor and not infrequently led to interactional problems. As we have seen, the paranoid is extremely sensitive to others' conscious and unconscious motives and equally insensitive to his own. Just so with Donald: he was totally unaware of his own behavior and appearance, as his attention was focused elsewhere. Because of his diverted attention, Donald manifested "a typical sign . . . of psychosis (which is), the individual's neglect of his appearance and hygiene" (Goffman 1963:27). Goffman's dual components of personal front are manner and appearance and their impression management counterparts politeness and decorum (Goffman 1959:108). Considering these dual com- ponents of personal front in relation to Donald (or any other psychotic), we can see why Donald's impression management will fail and, further, why paranoids' delusional systems are often self-fulfilling. Unfocused Interaction "In this realm of unfocused interaction, no one participant can be officially 'given the floor'; there is no official center of attention; . . , such a performance tends to be presented as if it were primarily for the benefit of everyone in the vicinity" (Goffman 1963:34). As we have seen, Donald did not perform in the usual, socially acceptable manner, He is a performer (in a show of unawareness) but the perform- ance is conspicuously transparent. Therefore, persons present in an unfocused interactional setting become acutely aware of Donald. He was clearly a person who did not adhere to the "obligation to convey certain information when in the presence of others and an obligation not to convey other impressions" (Goffman 1963:35). Donald became a focal point of scrutiny even before he had a chance to act. In an unfocused interactional situation, body idiom, "an idiom of individual appearance and gestures that tends to call forth in the actor what it calls forth in the others, the others drawn from those and only those who are present" (Goffman 1963:34), plays a significant part. Donald attributed meaning to all bodily gestures that he observed. He did not ceparate the gestures of others into classes of significant and nonsignificant (Mead 1964). Further, these gestures were not, as Goffman states, "Something less than significant symbolism" which fosters "an impression (that) must be maintained (which shows) that a margin of un- calculating spontaneous involvement has been retained in the act" (Goffman 1963:33-34), for Goffman speaks of normal interaction's symbol- ic behavior. All of this is understandable from Donald's point of view: "every- one is out to get me." In his psychotic condition, the actors in the world were all acting in direct relation to him. That being the case, even appropriate involvement shields (Goffman 1963:33-34) or absences were interpreted by Donald as secretive behavior. Meaningful absences included the withdrawal of generalized others to back region areas for the repair of a setting, or their persons, which were later to be O3 employed in front regions. An example of this can be seen in an unfocused situation where a hospital waiting room provided the setting. As Donald and I approached the waiting room, numbers of people could be observed situationally involved in waiting. Several of their number were also engaging in subordinate involvements, e.g., reading. Just after our entrance, a man got up and left the room. Donald's interpre- tation was this: "He was waiting here for us. He knew I was coming. He thinks I didn't see him behind that paper: He's probably calling the others now to let them know I am here." And when the man returned from the rest room, Donald said as he glared at him, "He thinks I don't know he just reported in. I know they have phones in the rest rooms." I later went to the rest room myself. Upon returning, I told Donald that there were no phones there. Donald retorted, "Oh, don't be stupid. He's got one of those small transmitters that he used in there." It is also important to note that Donald made definite distinctions as to the appropriateness of involvement shields. Donald himself did not use any such shields in public as he would have missed some of the activities of the plotters. In one instance, a man was reading a news- paper while standing on a street corner. Donald and I were across the street from him; we were waiting for the light to change so we could cross and continue on our errand. After observing the man for a few moments, Donald remarked,, "There is nothing important or interesting in the paper today (because)I read it." So it was obvious to Donald, "He's just acting like he's reading it. He's really following us and watching us." There were other occasions where Donald passed judgment on the validity of involvement shields, On each occasion, Donald was able to recognize his enemies by the fact that their shields against interaction- al involvement were inappropriate in his judgment. In this way, symbolic statements made in public places by people indicating that they did not wish to be engaged in conversation were construed by Donald as aggressive, hostile acts. By use of this interpretive procedure, Donald converted passive symbolic statements about non-involvement into aggressive, hos- tile acts of surveillance. Even in unfocused interaction, Donald's systematization of motives and intentions of actors created "situational insolence" (Goffman 1963: 42) because of Donald's acute awareness of the unfocused interactional behavior of others. Donald sensed, and rightly so, that the people around him were presenting a face which masked some of their real in- tentions. Donald recognized that even in unfocused interactions others were not being themselves; they were playing roles. Donald, however, misinterpreted the reality and intent of what it was others were trying to conceal. Focused Interaction Focused interaction is that which pertains to organized, directed and focused social interaction. This is interaction in which the actors 84 have their attention focused on one another and this focused attention is legitimatized by the underlying assumptions of such interactional encounters. Focused interaction of Donald was in part influenced back region behavior. Earlier we mentioned, as an act of ritual profanation of a non- present audience, Donald constantly carrying his wallet. When Donald extended this behavior into front region areas, it became an "occult involvement where the offender can supply a lively statement of the object of his special engrossment, which, however, persons present cannot credit. This retrospective aspect of the offense is often followed by the feeling that all of the offender's oncoming conduct is suspect. The kind of trust-in-the-other that is necessary if persons are to be in each other's presence and go on with their separate affairs can then be lost, and the offender ruined as a candidate for social intercourse. In a sense, then, a paranoid person is someone who has acted in such a way as to cause others to be suspicious and watchful of everything that he does; the persecutory feelings that result may be quite justified" (Goffman 1963:78). The occult involvement of Donald, the carrying of his wallet, also had other consequences. These consequences grew out of the fact that all of Donald's subsequent conduct was considered suspect. The suspicions of others were manifested in ways similar to those of Donald. That is, their suspicions were expressed by staring. Thus, another component of focused interaction, civil inattention, was consistently broken. "By according civil inattention, the individual implies that he has no reason to suspect the intentions of the others, be hostile to them, or wish to avoid them. (At the same time, in extending this courtesy he automatically opens himself up to a like treatment from others present.) This demonstrates that he has nothing to fear or avoid in being seen and being seen seeing, and that he is not ashamed of himself or of the place and company in which he finds himself" (Goffman 1963:84). Donald did not accord such polite inattention and so did not receive it. Also, when Donald stared at people for no apparent reason, the other people were aware that an "individual's gaze ought not to be guarded or averted or absent or defensively dramatic, as if 'something were going on"' (Goffman 1963:85). So the others observed by Donald were forced to make furtive glances at Donald to see if he was still staring at them. Donald took these glances to be proof that the other people are watching him. Goffman suggests proximity of interactants also influences inter- actional license. "The further they are from him, the more license they will feel to stare at him a little" (Goffman 1963:85). Donald recognized only that people were staring at him and, thus, regardless of the dis- tance between them, the staring represented spying to Donald. Often when people realized that Donald was staring at them, they took it to mean that a face engagement was expected because ". . . mutual glances ordinarily must be withheld if an encounter is to be avoided, for eye contact opens one up for face engagement" (Goffman 1963:95). I observed a good example of this interactional code being broken. This occurred while driving with Donald. We came to an intersection and at the red light I braked to a stop. In the adjacent lane, on the right of my car, was another car with a woman driver. While I was waiting for the light to change, I heard the woman in the other car say, "Do I know you?" Donald, who had been staring at the woman, retorted loudly, "I'm not stupid. I know you are following me' I ought to teach you a lesson." Needless to say, the woman was shocked. As tge light turned green, I sped away, leaving her to ponder what had happened. By making eye contact, Donald appears to be accessible (Goffman 1963:104) for interaction. But"cognitive and social recognition" (Goff- man 1963:113) are interpreted by Donald to have but one meaning; he is known because all the members of the plotting group "have been told to watch" for him. Often I observed Donald misconstrue the glances of others. And often, as in the example above, others looked at Donald because he looked or, more accurately, stared at them. CONCLUSIONS These few examples of interactional behavior provide insight into the kinds of interpretations and conclusions formed by a persecutory paranoid in social interactional performances. Since persecutory para- noid thinking is actually different only in degree and not in kind from normal thinking, one can easily visualize the paranoid in hypothetical situations. The paranoid's inability to be reflexive (reciprocity of egos), his inability to "correctly" interpret what he sees, his hypersensitivity to unintended meanings coupled with his insensitivity to his own appear- ance and behavior, his mistrust, suspicion, fear, and his unique reality, all play a part in preventing the paranoid from interacting "properly." It was the inability to interact in a "normal" way which made the subject of this paper a "proper" candidate for institutionalization and not the underlying psychological causes of his illness. His behavior was only symptomatic. It can be said, then, that a community's desire to institutionalize such a person is more a reaponse to symptomatology than a concern for the underlying causes of the mental condition. Donald's mode of interaction was quite justified given his world view. Through all of Donald's interactional difficulties, it became clear that what disturbed others most and what led to Donald's receipt of the label of madness was in large part his violation of the defenses of others' concepts of self as they were presented in interaction. Donald's condition was, in fact, seen as a threat to others only because he saw them as cruel, hostile, and persecuting; not because he actually threatened to do anyone physical harm. Donald was considered sick really because he could not accept as valid and at face value the managed impression of others. Our subject did not and could not proper- ly perform the interaction rituals which serve to foster others' 86 presentation of self in everyday life. For his failure, Donald was institutionalized for a period of several months. Fortunately, he was clearly in remission upon his release. The major failure of Donald was his obliviousness to the problematic character of social interactional encounters. Interaction is made pos- sible by the underlying assumption of a willingness to negotiate situa- tional definitions, a willingness to create them by a give and take, an exchange of meanings. For Donald, negotiation and, hence, exchange, was impossible. Donald developed and used a pre-existing definition which covered all social situations. Hence, his interaction became not only problematic, but-impossible. Perhaps other studies of social interaction do not so clearly show the consequences of an individual's failure in the social games of a given society. As Bailey's work (1971) and this study suggest, the acquisition and successful implementation of the assumptions which pro- vide the basis for interaction are not just matters of manipulating an inconsequential set of ideas regarding an ephemeral social etiquette. Rather, these are matters of knowing how to live in a society and how to remain a part of it. REFERENCES CITED Bailey, F. G., (ed.) 1971 Gifts and Poisons. New York: Schocken Books. Berreman, Gerald D. 1962 "Behind Many Masks: Ethnography and Impression Management in a Himalayan Village." Monograph No. 4, The Society for Applied Anthropology. Blumer, Herbert 1969 Symbolic Interactionism. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc. Cameron, Norman 1963 Personality Development and Psychopathology. Boston: Houghton- Mifflin Company. Freud, Sigmund 1938 The Basic Writings of Sigmund Freud. A. A. Brill, trans. New York: Random House. 1943 A General Introduction to Psychoanalysis. Joan Riviere, trans. Garden City, New York: Garden City Publishing Company. Freud, Anna 1966 The Ego and the Mechanisms of Defense. New York: Internation- al Universities Press (originally published 1936). Garfinkel, Harold 1967 Studies in Ethnomethodology. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, Inc. Goffman, Erving 1959 The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. Garden City, New York: Anchor Books, Doubleday and Company, Inc. 1963 Behavior in Public Places: Notes on the Social Organization of Gatherings. New York: The Free Press. 1967 Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face to Face Behavior. Garden City, New York: Anchor Books, Doubleday and Company. Laing, R. D. and Esterson, A. 1964 Sanity, Madness and the Family. Middlesex, England: Pelican Books, C. Nicholls and Company, Ltd. Mead, George Herbert 1964 On Social Psychology. Anseln Strauss, ed. Chicago: Unversity Chicago Press. Schutz, Alfred 1967 Collected Papers: The Problem of Social Reality. Maurice Natanson, ed. The Hague, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.