3 Patterns and Functions of Grooming Behavior among the Common Indian Langur Monkey James J. McKenna For many primate species grooming is a predomin- ant and socially important behavior pattern diverse both in the form it can take and the social situations within which it is initiated. While not the only behavior that often cuts across different age and sex classes to promote cohesion, the tremendous amount of time and energy invested by some species in this activity suggests that as a bond-building mechanism social grooming has played an auxiliary if not complemen- tary role in the evolution of primate sociality. Among primates social grooming is thought by in- vestigators to provide useful information on many dif- ferent aspects of group life. Grooming data has been used to measure developmental processes in the mother-infant relationship (Hinde 1974, Kaufman and Rosenblum 1969), to document the growing net- work of attachments and subgroup formations within and among different primate groups (Simonds 1973, 1974; Furuya 1957; Kummer 1968), and as an exam- ple of parental investment (Trivers 1974, Hrdy 1976). While Sade (1965) and Lindberg (1973) demonstrated how grooming patterns reinforce social relationships and, in some cases, regulate behavior, Chance and Jolly (1970) suggested that within private societies the direction of grooming can be used as one criterion to delineate "attention structures" and to assess the de- gree of social centrality of leader individuals around which others are organized. More recently, Boese (1976) used data collected on preferred grooming clusters to propose that West African baboons (Papio papio) exhibit harem units based upon sexual but not social exclusivity. Although an impressive number of works dealing primarily with the social functions of grooming exist, there is not unanimous agreement that grooming be- havior has been interpreted and dealt with correctly, nor that its function has been entirely understood. Alexander (1974) argues that not enough attention has been given to the disease-controlling functions of grooming, particularly since grooming may have orig- inally functioned only as a parasite-controlling mechanism and not as an accomodation to group liv- ing. To varying degrees Alexander's position receives support from Hutchins and Barash (1976) and Free- land (1976) who strongly argue that a much closer look at the relationship between disease pressures and so- cial behavior is warranted. This paper examines the forms, the contexts, and the functions of social grooming behavior among the common Indian langur money (Presbytis entellus). These data suggest, particularly with respect to the question of functions, that while grooming no doubt serves both social and hygienic functions, it does so in a context apparently dictated -by social needs. It is con- cluded that social grooming among langur monkeys is highly integrated into almost all aspects of group life and, while not a reliable behavioral index to status differentials between group members, it is important in assessing role complexes. Materials and Methods The study group was housed in a dome-shaped mesh enclosure approximately 35 feet high by 45 feet in diameter. Starting from the enclosure floor six dif- ferent levels of sitting bars and platforms, interspersed every three to five feet, allowed proper social and spatial refuge. An L-shaped mesh partition support- ing plywood sitting platforms was situated in the center of the enclosure. According to Jay's (1965) field data, the population structure included an adult male (about ten years of age with extracted canines and musculature fully de- veloped); eight adult females (all at least silx years of age, primiparous or multiparous); four immature juvenile females (all between the ages of two and four years of age); four immaturejuvenile males (with mus- culature not fully developed and incompletely erupted canines); and, finally, two dependent infants less than eight months old. To collect data on the form, frequencies, and dura- tions of behavioral patterns for group members three different sampling techniques were utilized: focal be- havior samples, focal animal samples, and behavior spot checks. A focal behavior sample involved a fifteen minute observation period wherein all grooming ac- tivities were recorded and notation was made of which animal initiated the grooming interaction, which other animal received it, its context, and what part of the anatomy was groomed. During a focal animal sample all behavior exhibited by and toward one individual (the focal animal) during a fifteen minute observation period was recorded. The duration of the behavior, the actor, the receiver and the participants were noted. Behavior spot checks involved a random scan and periodic survey of the momentary activities of each individual at the time of observation (see Altmann 1974). Combining the three sampling procedures a total of 1500 observation hours were completed. Tabulation of the data followed the procedure used by Lindberg (1973), in which a grooming bout was considered to be any paired occurrence of grooming involving a recognizable actor and receiver not inter- rupted by a third animal or by a major relocation of one of the participants. Reciprocal grooming (animal A grooms B, then, animal B grooms A) was considered as two different bouts. If three or more animals groomed simultaneously (animal A grooms animal B while animal C grooms A), each grooming dyad was scored separately. If more than ten seconds passed between bouts, or it a grooming sequence was termi- nated but begun again after an individual invited a partner to resume grooming, then this was scored as a second grooming bout even though the participants had previously groomed during the sampling period. Forms of Grooming Although the contexts were varied, three types of grooming were recognized for captive Hanuman lan- gurs: 1) relaxed social grooming, 2) tension-reducing grooming, and 3) self-grooming (see table 1). Groom- ing interactions involving more than one animal and not accompanied by aggressive or tense vocalizations, postures, and/or facial expressions (grimaces) were scored as bouts of relaxed social grooming. In contrast, any bout preceded by an embrace, a social present, aggressive behavior, a social or sexual mount, or a sexual solicitation involving tense vocalizations and/or facial expressions was scored as a tension-reducing groom. Finally, self-grooming was scored when an in- dividual licked or picked through its own skin, hair, or teeth. Table 1. Taxonomy of the forms, contexts, and percentages of different grooming behaviors observed among captive Hanuman langurs (P. entellus) As Table 1 reveals, of the observed 1907 initiated grooming behaviors 60% involved relaxed social grooming, 35% involved tension-reducing grooming, and 4% involved solitary or self-grooming. Utilizing Sparks (1967) distinctions, I computed that, of the 60% (n= 1146) classified as relaxed social grooming, 4% took the form of mutual grooming (animal A and B groom each other simultaneously); 53% involved allo-grooming (animal A grooms animal B); 42% in- volved reciprocal grooming (animal A grooms animal B, then, animal B grooms animal A); and, finally, in less than 1% of the relaxed social grooming total the form of grooming was unclear, since observation began after the animals had initiated the interaction. Aggressive behavior preceded 63% of tension- reducing grooming bouts (667 or 35% of all social grooming bouts observed). Ventral-ventral embracing was the second most frequent behavior to precede tension-reducing grooming with 22% of the total. Sexual solicitations, social mountings, and social pre- senting were roughly equal in eliciting grooming re- sponses (2.5%, 1%, and 4.2% respectively). The third and least frequently observed form of grooming among captive langurs was self-grooming. This form of solitary skin care occurred in 4% of the total number of grooming behaviors observed. Al- though spatial and, therefore, social isolation often preceded self-grooming, it was certainly not the only condition in which it was initiated. Self-grooming often occurred between reciprocal grooming sessions and/or following or preceding allo-grooming by either one of the participants. Frequency and Distribution of Grooming Patterns by Age and Sex Class Figure 1 shows the percentages by which age and sex classes acted and received social grooming (relaxed and tension-reducing grooming combined). Based upon 94 fifteen-minute samples on each of the adults and immatures (excluding the neonates) and 399 focal behavior hours (1,596 fifteen-minutes observation periods) I found that during any one sample period an average of 3.6 individuals could be found either as actors or receivers in grooming interactions. The adult male was involved in more grooming interactions than any other individual (331 or 10% of the total and received approximately twice as much grooming as he returned. As an age and sex class the adult females partici- pated in the actor role more than twice as frequently as they did in the receiver role (actor to receiver ratio for adult females was 1/2.2). While a t-test comparing the differences between the mean frequency of initiated grooming bouts per animal per class showed that adult females initiated and acted grooming at rates signific- antly different (p. <01) than either immature males or females, there is much variation within age and sex classes which deserves attention. When adult females are treated as a class of individuals grouped on the basis of age and sex statuses the standard deviation was 46 and the coefficient of variation was extremely high (21.3). This figure assumes more meaning when it is considered that one adult female (Goldie) initiated far 4 Grooming Behavior SOCIAL (96%) SELF (4%) RELAXED (60%) TENSION-REDUCING (35%) ____ la 0 _ - 0q a az3. ': :, t)E .XD vi.u :M> . W:: W6Wv ...  1f3ig. $d>>Xe7 S FXrXilg4 f .':A:%g: : aD.Gg.Sk.:RS .?>S S sex class boundaries the grooming activities of adult females may function to enhance group integration especially since their grooming activities include immature males whose positions in the group are socially tenuous. Social grooming solicitations and the contexts in which grooming occurred were tremendously di- verse. While the majority of grooming bouts were described as relaxed, tension-reducing grooming which was recognized when one or both of the groom- ing participants exhibited tense facial expressions or vocalizations occurred after aggressive interactions, before and after social mounts, during sexual and infant-caretaking activities, following social presents and after ventral-ventral embracing. In addition, it was found that although grooming invitations or sol- icitations were not often a part of a tension-reducing sequence as they were a part of the relaxed social grooming bouts, in the majority of cases it was the receiver of aggression who acted the grooming while the aggressor turned away and received it. The rela- tionship between grooming and aggression proved to be an interesting one since a positive correlation coef- ficient was computed. As the frequency of grooming increased between individuals so did aggression or vice-versa. The computation of this statistic reveals the value of considering context since rarely (if at all) did aggression follow grooming behavior between participants. In most instances aggression preceded it. Hence, grooming seemed to promote the restora- tion of peaceful relationships between individuals and the cessation of hostilities. As an appeasement mechanism the grooming behavior of captive langur monkeys cannot be underestimated. With respect to the strong statements put forth by Freeland (1976), Alexander (1974) and Hutchins and Barash (1976) on the disease-controlling functions of grooming behavior these data can neither prove, nor disprove their contentions. It was, however, stressed that among langurs the part of the anatomy exposed by one animal to another is primarily determined by the nature of the interaction preceding the grooming bout. Inaccessible areas of the body were groomed most often after aggressive encounters. More specifi- cally, ventral-ventral grooming sessions, i.e. groom- ing interactions that involved the animals facing each other in which eye-contact was sustained, occurred most often during relaxed social grooming bouts. During tense grooming encounters ventral-dorsal body orientations permitting the avoidance of eye- contact between grooming dyads were more com- mon. These facts are relevant to understanding the circumstances in which inaccessible parts of the body are sometimes groomed. While this grooming may function in the long run to decrease disease poten- attending to it and providing tactile enjoyment while the recipient of aggression and the actor of the groom- ing manipulates an aggression-receiving situation into a peaceful one. With the present data in mind some tentative con- clusions can be offered. Among Hanuman langurs social grooming is intimately fused with almost all aspects of social life and can be used to manipulate social situations and to establish important social al- liances. The argument that grooming originally evolved as a parasite-controlling mechanism appears simplistic and ignores the fact that natural selection works not upon one behavior but on complexes of behavior and makes continuous compromises on the efficacy of each adaptive strategy. The "complimen- tarity" of grooming behavior as defined by Weber wherein active/passive, dominant/subordinant roles are ascribed to adult male and female langurs is, likewise, unsatisfactory particularly since status dif- ferentials are so fluid in langur society. No doubt social grooming performs dualistic social and hygienic functions but given the social base from which colobines evolved it may prove unwise to separate the two, especially since both immediate and long terms benefits are mutually derived. tials, the actions of both participants (actor and re- ceiver) provide immediate social rewards; the actor of the aggression is appeased by the attacked animal 11 REFERENCES CITED Alexander, Richard D. 1974 The Evolution of Social Behavior. Annual Review of Ecology and Systema- tics 5 325-383. Altmann, Jeanne 1974 Observational Study of Be- havior: Sampling Methods. Behavior 49:227-267. Boese, Gilbert K. 1976 Social Behavior and Ecological Considerations of West African Baboons (Papio papio). In Socioecology and Psychology of Pri- mates. Russell Tuttle, ed., Mouton: Chicago, Pp. 205-230. Chance, Michael, and Clifford Jolly, 1970 Social Groups of Monkeys, Apes and Men. New York: E.P. Dutton. Curtin, Richard 1975 Socioecology of the Common Langur, Presbytus entellus, in Nepal Himalaya. Ph.D. dissertation, Univ. Calif., Berkeley. Freeland, 1976 Pathogens and the Evolution of Pri- mate Sociality. Biotropica 8 (1): 12-24. Furuya, Y. 1957 Grooming Behavior in WildJapanese Monkeys. In Primates, (1): 47-68. Hinde, Robert 1974 Biological Basis of Human Social Behavior. New York: McGraw Hill. Hladik, C.M. 1975 Ecology, Diet, and Social Pattern- ing in Old and New World Primates. In Socioecol- ogy and Psychology of Primates. RusselTuttle, ed., The Hague: Mouton, Pp. 3-35. Hrdy, Sarah 1976 Care and Exploitation of Nonhu- man Primate Infants by Conspecifics Other than Mother. In Advances in the Study of Behavior, 6. New York: Academic Press, Pp. 101-158. Hutchins, Michael and David Barsh, 1976 Grooming in Primates: Implications for its Utilitarian Func- tion. Primates 17(2): 145-150. Jay, P. 1965 The Common Langur of North India. In I. DeVore ed., Primate Behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Pp. 197-249. Kaufman, I.C. and Leonard Rosenblum 1969 The Waning of the Mother-Infant bond in Two Species of Macaque. In B.M. Foss, ed., Determinants of Infant Behavior. IV. London: Mentuen, Pp. 41-60. Kummer, Hans 1968 Group Techniques of Ecological Adaptations. Chicago: Aldine. Lindberg, Charles 1973 Grooming Behavior as a Reg- ulator of Social Interactions in Rhesus Monkeys.In Behavioral Regulators of Behavior in Primates. R. Carpenter, ed., Bucknell University Press. McKenna,James 1975 Social Roles and Behavior of Seventeen Captive Hanuman Langur monkeys (Pres bytis entellus). Ph.D. dissertation, University of Oregon. Sade, Donald 1965 Some Aspects of Parent-Offspring and Sibling Relations in a Group of Rhesus Mon- keys, with a Discussion of Grooming. Am.J. Phys. Anthrop. 23: 1-18. Simonds, Paul 1973 Outcast Males and Social Struc- ture Among Bonnet Macaques (Macaca radiata). Am.J. Phys. Anthrop. 38: 599-604. 1974 The Social Primates. New York: Harper and Row. Sparks,J. 1967 Allogrooming in Primates: A Review. In Primate Ethology, D. Morris, ed., Pp. 148-175. Chicago: Aldine Trivers, Robert 1974 Parent-Offspring Conflict. Amer. Zool. 11:219-264. Weber, Inge 1973 Tactile Communication Among Free-ranging Langurs. Am. J. Phys. Anthrop. 38:481-486. 1 2