7 Central California Archaeology: The Concepts of Pattern and Aspect David A. Fredrickson (1982, revised 1984) Introduction HE CLASSIFICATION OF archaeological assemblages is now and always has been one of the most troublesome tasks for the scholar, and yet dealing with the theoretical bases of such classifica- don is a task that most of us tend to side-step or put off until another day. Randy Milliken reminds me that we tend to forget that we are anthropologists and, because of this, we often treat archaeological materials as objects apart from the people who were responsible for their deposition. We seem rarely to draw upon our anthrpological knowledge of human behavior and cultural complexity. The work of Willey and Phillips (1958), in my estimation, is still the most useful exploration of con- cepts employed in archaeological classification. I see no problem in accepting the bulk of their basic defini- tions, and indeed urge for the sake of more explicit communication that these definitions be accepted by others as well. According to Willey and Phillips (1958:22-24), the "phase" is: an archaeological unit possessing traits suffi- ciently characteristic to distinguish it from all odherunits similarlyconceived, whetherofthe same or other cultures or civilizations, spa- tially limited to the order of magnitude of a locality or region and chronologically limited to a relatively brief interval of time. Problems do occur in operationalizing the concept. The absence of specific criteria for defining phases is shown by the comment that: a phase may be anything from a thin level in a site reflecting no more than a briefencamp- ment to a prolonged occupation of a large number of sites distributed over a region of very elastic proportion (Wiley and Phillips 1958:22-24). Further, as additional data are compiled, phases may be subdivided into smaller units, which themselves may be either equivalent phases or groupings of subphases. Again there is an absence of explicit criteria for such subdivision. We are familiar with the construction of local sequences, often based upon the excavation of a single complex site or a number of related sites in a single locality. Local sequences are most commonly the product of a single worker, or a small group of cooperating individuals, who have firsthand knowl- edge of the materials which they place into the local sequence. By contrast, the regional sequence, such as ones in the North Coast Ranges or the upper Sacra- mento Valley and environs, requires correlations be- tween various local sequences. It is instructive to 76 Toward a New Taxonomic Frameworkfor Central California again review at lengti the comments of Willey and Phillips (1958:26-29) on this issue: Ideally, the archaeologists of a region come together in a harmonious session where a careful matching of local sequences produces a new sequence of larger scope. Actually this happy event occurs but rarely. What more often hapens is that phases and local se- quences gain in scope by a sort of osmosis. They flow outward, so to speak, often pro- pelled by their onginators, uniting to them- selvestheirweakercorrelatesoverawidening circle. The process is necessarily accom- plished by a progressive generalization of definition until much of the original useful- ness to research is impaired. I provide these citations from an earlier genera- tion to remind us that the classification of archaeo- logical assemblages has never been an easy task and, implicit in the Willey and Phillips analysis, there is seldom agreement among researchers who are sepa- rately (or even cooperatively) attempting to organize archaeological data into meaningful spatial or tempo- ral units. The Central Califomia sequence identified in Bulletin 2 of the Sacramento Junior College (Lillard, Heizer and Fenenga 1939), and later refined and extended by Richard Beardsley (1954), is a good case in point. Gerow (1954; Gerow with Force 1968), Ragir (1972), and Fredrickson (1973,1974a), as well as others from time to time, have offered critiques of the Central California classification. Clearly disagreements occur for a number of different reasons. On the formal level we can see from the Willey and Phillips definition of a phase that there is no standard established for defining the minimum degree of similarity between assemblages to warrant grouping them together. There is no definition of the nature of the differences which would warrant the separation of assemblages into different units. If archaeological assemblages are examined employing the direct historical approach, with reference to known ethnographic data, the logical problem of specialized assemblages of the same community occurring at different locations and otherwise incomplete assem- blages comes to light. In fonnal tenns such assem- blages could be grouped into separate phases repre- senting different archaeological "cultures." It is also evident that when we attempt to correlate local se- quences (often incompletely and poorly defined) within a region, the absence of adequately defined criteria impedes the process and hinders agreement between different workers. Even when workers accept the same taxonomic framework, which they rarely seem to do, questions such as the one above remain, i.e., do contrasting assemblages represent different activities of the same group or the basic assemblage of contrast- ing communities? In the transition zone between adjacent regions, regardless of how criteria are estab- lished, it often appears arbitrary as to whether a parficular phase is grouped with one regional se- quence or another. Archaeologists are dealing widt the same kinds of problems thatethnologists attempted to deal within refining the culture area concept. Unfortunately in archaeology, small sample sizes, the lack of compa- rable materials, and the uneven quality of the data make meaningful statistical correlations difficult if not impossible, so that work analogous to that done by Driver and Coffin (1975), and during an even earlier generation by Klimek (1935), is not feasible in most archaeological regions, at least in Califomia. Cri- tiques applied to the culture area concept in general anthropology apply many times over in archaeology. Binford (1965) has discussed some of these problems within the contrasting frameworks of the nonnative and processual approaches. He rightfully points out that culture is not shared but is a system in which persons participate. "Individuals and social units are articulated by means of various institutions into broader units having different levels of corporate inclusiveness" (Binford 1965:205). Since culture is not a univariate phenomenon, it is methodologically unsound to simply group assemblages together with respect to numbers of similarities and differences. For example, if shell beads and omaments dominate the known assemblage, such groupings would not iden- tify communities in the discrete sense but rather a portion of an exchange network that may include a wide range of separate communities. Binford (1965:208-209) suggested that there are at least three major types of "broad cultural alignments" that may vary independently of one another-the tradition, interaction sphere, and adaptive area. For Binford, tradition refers to "demonstrable The Concepts of Pattern and Aspect 77 continuity through time in the fonnal properties of locally manufactured craft items," or "stylistic vari- ability." Interaction spheres are "areal matrices of regular and institudonally maintained intersocietal articulation," which may crosscut both traditions and culture areas. An adaptive area is one "which exhib- its the common occurrence of artifacts used primarily with coping directly with the physical environment." Binford (1965:209) relates the adaptive area to the culture area concept, excluding stylistic attributes from the definition. He argues that these three spheres should be studied as independent variables if we are to understand more fully the operation of cultural pro- cesses. While Binford's critique of the nonnative ap- proach has heuristic value and the fornulations dis- cussed above have considerable value, the major problems in classification of assemblages are still not addressed. General concepts that appear to make sense are suggested, but no clue has been offered with respect to operationalizing them. For example, how do we compare the stylistic traditions of one local sequence with those of another sequence? Binford appears to ignore the problem, and, indeed, the "spheres" suggested by Binford are themselves mul- tivariate phenomena. It is likely that an "interaction sphere," for example, consists of a variable numberof subsystems none necessarily covarying with the oth- ers. Recent ethnographers have emphasized the vari- ability in individual behavior within communities, thus adding more doubt regarding the bases for taxo- nomic grouping of archaeological materials. Nonetheless, we archaeologists must deal with the problem of organizing massive amounts of infor- mation into groupings which have meaning to us, despite apparent lack of congruence with the real world. Inmy doctoral dissertation (Fredrickson 1973), drawing heavily on the work of Jim Bennyhoff, I suggested a series of concepts to deal with temporal and spatial classification in Califomia. These con- cepts were based upon those of Wiley and Phillips (1958) but with some alterations that attempted to reconcile normative and processual approaches. The reconciliation was not explicitly done, and, upon review with the benefit of hindsight, I did not fully recognize the multivariate nature of culture. The system which I proposed has been most extensively used in the North Coast Ranges, but it has recently gained currency throughout the state (e.g. Moratto 1984). In this essay, I focus on the concepts of "pattem," "aspect," and "period," all of which were first intro- duced to northem California archaeology in 1973 (Fredrickson 1973). At that time, I (Fredrickson 1973:118-19) wrote that a pattern was characterized by: "(a) similar technological skills and devices (spe- cific cultural items); (b) similar economic modes (production, distribution, consumption), including par- ticipation in trade networks and practices surrounding wealth (often inferential); and (c) similar mortuary and ceremonial practices." I stated that local varia- tion in a pattem may sometimes be extreme depend- ing upon "(a) abundance and nature of specific envi- ronmental resources; (b) regional specialization and elaborations; (c) degree of cultural and geographic marginality; (d) influence of neighboring pattems." I originally conceived of both pattern and aspect as representing different levels of interaction spheres. The aspect represented direct community interaction, and the pattem represented more widespread inter- action, perhaps comparable to what might be called an "effective interaction sphere," as contrasted with the "minimum interaction sphere" represented by the aspect and the "maximum interaction sphere," repre- sented, for example, by trade horizons, which may cut across the boundaries of adjacent archaeological ar- eas. While I still hold this view, I believe we can further refine the concepts, borrowing from Binford. At this time, I offer a refinement in the definition of pattern, more in keeping with the "adaptive sphere" of Binford. I retain criteria of similar technological skill and devices but reject as a necessary criteria the similarity of mortuary and ceremonial practces. This is not to say, however, that such linkages do not occur fiom time to time. The concept of economic modes which I employed in 1973 is far too broad and, in fact, represents a series of distinctly separate activi- ties and processes. I would retain similarity with respect to the means of production (and probably consumption), but distribution, especially as it relates to exchange systems and wealth practices, must be kept apart from the pattern concept. Although I keep 78 Toward a New Taxonomic Frameworkfor Central California exchange outside of the pattem concept, I think it likely that there is a level of exchange, or interaction, which is represented within a pattem. I see no need for any significant change in the qualifications regarding local and regional variation with a pattem. North coastal California pattems identified to date include Gunther (of Northwest Califomia) and Augustine (of Central California), both of the Emer- gent period; Berkeley (of Central Califomia, emanat- ing from the Bay Area and with historical origins possibly from the south coast); Borax Lake; and the still-provisional Post (see Fredrickson 1984; Fredrick- son and White 1988). Discussion is still underway regarding the nature and definition of assemblages that appear to represent one or more patterns interme- diate between Borax Lake and Gunther and Borax Lake and Augustine. Each pattem can be conceived as representing an adaptive sphere strongly marked by the common occurrence of artifacts used primarily in coping directly with the physical environment. We should recall that although culture is indeed a multi- variate phenomenon, independent variables may in- deed(butnotnecssarily)covarysimultaneously. Thus, we do at times observe burial modes changing simul- taneously with extractive technology. The pattem concept can be seen to represent a "lumper's delight." The phase and subphase, by contrast, are each a "splitter's delight," and, since each is ideally based upon detailed analyses of stylistic elements, each representsanequivalentofBinford's'taditionsphere." Having now borrowed from Binford, we are still left with the problem of dealing with variation in the pattern, or adaptive sphere. Such variation can be identified on the basis of stylistic elements (Binford's traditions). However, phases and subphases represent the smallest archaeological units that can be identified and may, if we are fortunate, actually represent the equivalent of a community. Following Willey and Phillips (1958), the geographic space through which a given phase or subphase can be traced would be a locality. In practice, however, we can rarely distin- guish one locality (in the sense of a local tribelet territory) from another. Forpurposes here-acknowl- edging Jim Bennyhoff as the originator of the idea- I refer to the space encompassed by a phase or subphase that canbe traced and canbe referred to as a "distnrct." As a working hypothesis, Bennyhoff (see Bennyhoff 1977), has proposed that districts equate with linguis- tic communities. While ideally every politically au- tonomous community should be represented in the archaeological record by a distinct sequence of phases, in practice-based on ethnographic and protohistoric parallels-the district is most commonly made up of a series of interacting communities, communities that are probably linked through internarriage and kin- ship. If we examine Yellen's (1 977) data from Africa, and data recently generated by Randy Milliken from Mission Registers, such communities may be actively and constantly exchanging personnel. As we view the archaeological data from the Califomia culture area, the major exchange networks themselves allow the suggestion that we are in need of a taxonomic grouping to deal with variability not expressed within the pattern and aspect concepts. We can observe a level of assemblage similarity that appears to fall between these two concepts as they have been applied to date. Conceiving of the aspect as a "minimum interaction sphere," as noted above, has limited our synthesizing ability. It may be better conceived of as an "internediate interaction sphere." In 1973 I defined an aspect as being equivalent at any one time to a phase, and over a longer period of time by a sequence of phases. With the aspect expanded to the intennediate level, at any given time slot, it can be made up of perhaps several synchronous but some- what contrasting phases. A specific phase, then, is situated within a specific locality. Several similar phases in adjoining localities would be grouped into an aspect, with the localities grouped into a district. To facilitate communication prior to definition of specific phases and aspects, I further suggest a concept that I refer to as a "style-area" be adopted to deal with this mid-range variability. How the concept is to be employed to distinguish the multivariate natureof archaeological materials remains tobe worked out. I can conceive of the concept being employed in each of two major ways: (1) by a qualifier expressing geographic spread, best applied to assemblage simi- larities on a level intennediate between pattem and aspect (but not necessarily limited to this level), and (2) by a stylistic qualifier representing the geographic distribution of a particular stylistic element. For example, the "Tehama" materials discussed by Elaine Sundahl and similar materials discussed by Bill Dryer The Concepts of Pattern and Aspect 79 might be grouped together into a "Southem Cascade style-area." On the other hand, the geographic area in which the indented base, wide-stemmed projectile point (spear point or knife) is found could be referred to as the "indented base, wide-stemmed point style area," or more simply, the "wide-stem style-area." This practice could reduce our tendency to equate the wide-stem point necessarily with the Borax Lake pattem. Further discussion regarding this concept, or an alternative method of referring to the middle level of cultural grouping, could prove productive. I do suggest that we pay more heed to the multi- variate nature of culture, adding such focus to the synthesizing tasks in which we are already involved. Although I have pointed out the lack of satisfactory criteria for defining the several taxonomic units, I have not attempted to deal with the problem. Over- coming the problem may well take us back to an "'old- fashioned" approach, that of plotting geographic and temporal distributions of discrete artifact types and subtypes, rather than focusing all our energies upon the assemblage alone. We might call this the testing of the multivariate hypothesis. Such an approach may help us refrain from reifying our constructs, and of assuming similarities when we apply the same name to assemblages from different locations. Our state of knowledge should now encourage us to entertain multiple working hypotheses. I cannot overempha- size my perception that we are at an ideal place in data collection and analysis. We may now take a step back and be critical (and subsequently creative) regarding methodological issues. Our comparative skills and methods, for sure, need honing. I have recently been made aware of yet another dimension of assemblage distribution which is not encompassed by the taxonomic system as originally conceived or as it might be modified according to the suggestions above. Greg White has infonned me of the apparent coexistence of contemporaneous but contrasting assemblages within the same locality. What appears to be persistence of the milling stone assemblage in Lake County is found cotenninously in localities containing Houx assemblages. Brian Wickstrom has noted the identical phenomenon in the Santa Rosa locality, with a milling stone assemblage co-occurring with Berkeley assemblages. Both of these examples could be similar to the "Tehama Pat- tem" as described by Clewett and Sundahl (1990). It may be that archaeology must lose the unity of the Califomia culture area as defined on the basis of ethnographic traits. In that event, Augustine and Shasta would become synchronous pattems and Tehama, then, would become an aspect of Shasta or Augustine, or even be elevated to the pattem level itself. In the North Coast Ranges, Houx would be- come a pattem separate from Berkeley. The major problem with this approach is that it encourages classificatory fragmentation, but perhaps that cost is in keeping with the benefits that might be accrued. As for the second problem, we know on the basis of ethnographic data that contrasting cultures can indeed occupy different niches within the same habi- tat (i.e. within the same or overlapping geographic ranges). It is clear to me that the Central Califomia taxonomy as I have discussed it is inadequate in treating this circumstance.